Th 3:00-5:50 PM in Tempe, COOR
3301 ED 236
Office hours: Thursday after class or by appointment
Topic
Legal probabilism is an ongoing research program that aims to harness the powers of probability theory to analyze, model and improve the evaluation of evidence and the process of decision-making in trial proceedings. Despite its promise, significant hurdles exist in carrying out this program, and many have criticized it. These critiques range from difficulties in assessing the probability of someone’s criminal or civil liability to the dehumanization of trial decisions to misconstruing how the process of evidence evaluation and decision-making takes place in trial proceedings. The seminar examines these current debates and their relevance to larger questions about AI and the human future.
Objectives
This course is meant for advanced undergraduates and graduate students. You will become familiar with current debates in philosophy, law and computer science about legal reasoning, evidence and uncertainty with a focus on questions about the use of probability, statistics and AI. You will sharpen your critical thinking skills, in reading and writing, for the analysis of theoretical concepts and their application to current legal questions. You will read academic papers in philosophy, law and computer science and develop an appreciation for their distinctive contributions.
Course Materials
Readings and other course materials are available on this website.
For readings covered by copyright, please check the Canvas page of this
course.
Course materials are divided into “primary” and “additional”. You are
only expected to study the primary ones, but I recommend that you have a
look at the additional materials for at least one or two weeks.
There is no textbook, but these two references can be useful as a guide:
Urbaniak and Di Bello (2021), Legal Probabilism
Fundamentals of Probability and Statistical Evidence in Criminal Proceedings
Requirements
Participation
Please attend class regularly and participate (10% of your grade). This is a “seminar style” course. You are expected to take an active role in the discussions. Please study the assigned materials before class and be ready to discuss them.
Assignments
You should write ten pass/fail précis, one pass/fail take-home exam, and three graded essays or a research paper (90% of your grade).
(a) Précis
Every week please write a one-page précis of one of the papers assigned for that week. Unless otherwise noted, the précis should describe: (a) topic of the paper; (b) main thesis (or main theses, if there are more than one); (c) supporting arguments; (d) objections to these arguments, complications or difficulties that the author considers (if any). Submit your précis each week through Canvas before the beginning of class. You should receive a PASS in at least ten précis, or else a full letter grade will be subtracted from your final grade: A would become B; B would become C; etc.
(b) Exam
You will take an exam that tests your understanding of the basics of probability theory. The exam is take-home and pass/fail.
(c) Threee essays - option 1
There will be three main graded assignments for this course, 5 pages each.
I. Write a critical essay that outlines the key tenets of legal probabilism and the criticisms leveled against it. Your essay should begin with a summary of the key tenets of legal probabilism (1-2 pages). After that, you should apply legal probabilism to a toy example, say a stylized court case (1-2 pages). Finally, you should raise problems and difficulties for legal probabilism (1-2- pages). Please draw from the readings and demonstrate thoughtful engagement with them.
II. After doing independent research, identify a disputed factual issue in a court case and analyze it using probability theory. Your essay should serve as case study that illustrates the advantages and challenges of applying probability theory to a real legal case. The first part of your essay (1-2 pages) should describe informally the key items of evidence in the case and the disputed facts. The second part (3-4 pages) should apply probability theory to analyze and weigh the evidence.
III. Write a philosophical argumentative essay that examines the topic “Legal Probabilism: Opportunities and Challenges”. In examining this topic, you should draw from the course readings though I do not expect you to cover all of them. You may (but need not) build on the case study you discussed in your second essay. Your essay can be primarily negative, e.g., you argue that the challenges are insurmountable. Your essay can also be more positive, e.g., you describe the challenges and propose ways to address them within legal probabilism.
(c) Research paper - option 2
If you are a grad student or advanced undergrad with research experience, you may combine the three 5 page essays into one 15-20 page research paper. The research paper should engage closely with a subset of the course materials including the additional ones. It is neither necessary nor recommended that you use materials outside those already included in the course materials.
Please come talk to me before you start working on the research paper.
Schedule
8/22
Week 1: The Collins case
Probability and statistical methods can be used in trial proceedings for evaluating the evidence presented against the defendant. Sometimes the evidence itself is presented in the form of probabilities and statistics. This numerical presentation is often useful but can also lead to confusions, exaggerations and errors. To illustrate some of the problems, we will start by discussing the infamous case People v. Collins (1968). This case started the law and probability debate in the United States back in the 70ies. Along the way, we will learn the basics of probability theory.
Guides, handouts, notes, etc.
Primary readings
People v. Collins (1968)
Finkelstein and Levin (2001), Statistics for Lawyers, Sec. 3.1-3.2
Additional readings
- Finkelstein and Fairley (1970), A Bayesian Approach to Identification Evidence
8/29
Week 2: Match evidence
Match evidence, such as DNA and fingerprint evidence, offers the clearest example of how probability and statistics can help to weigh evidence. We will discuss how match evidence works, and the debates about how it should be presented at trial. We will continue learning about probability theory.
Guides, handouts, notes, etc.
Primary readings
People v. Rush (1995)
Wasserman (2004), Forensic DNA Typing
Additional readings
Haavik(2016), It’s a match. Or is it?
Koehler (1993), Error and Exaggeration in the Presentation of DNA Evidence
Zabell (2005), Fingerprint Evidence
9/5
Week 3: Unlikely coincidences
If a suspiciously high number of people die in a hospital when a nurse is on duty—much higher than when other nurses are on duty—is this evidence that the nurse is culpable, or was this just a coincidence? We will look at two infamous cases, Lucia de Berk and Sally Clark, in which statistics about unlikely coincidences were used—actually, abused!—to conclude that a wrongdoing occurred.
Guides, handouts, notes, etc.
Primary readings
R v Clark (2003)
Derksen (2007), The Fabrication of Fact
Additional readings
Meester and others (2006), On the (Ab)use of Statistics in the Legal Case Against the Nurse Lucia de B
Fienberg and Kaye (1991), Legal and Statistical Analysis of some Mysterious Clusters
Dotto, Gill and Mortera (2022), Statistical Analyses in the Case of an Italian Nurse Accused of Murdering Patients
Gill, Groeneboom and de Jong (2019), Elementary Statistics on Trial: the Case of Lucia de Berk
9/12
Week 4: Profiling
There is little doubt that match evidence should be used at trial as evidence of guilt, for example, when the defendant matches the crime traces. But what about evidence that people with certain traits are extremely likely to a commit a crime? For example, data show that people who committed a sexual offense are very likely to commit it again. How should this information be used at trial?
Guides, handouts, notes, etc.
Primary readings
Redmayne (2003), The Relevance of Bad Character
Pundik (2020), Predictive Evidence and Unpredictable Freedom
Additional readings
Aviv (2013), The Science of Sex Abuse
Di Bello and O’Oneil (2020), Profile Evidence, Fairness, and the Risks of Mistaken Convictions
United States v. Vue (1994)
9/19
Take-home exam due
Week 5: Probability of guilt?
We have seen how different types of evidence can be evaluated using probability and statistics. But what is the goal of this evaluation? Some think that the goal is to arrive at an assessment of the probability that the defendant is guilty, but there are also different probabilistic measures of the value of the evidence.
Guides, handouts, notes, etc.
Primary readings
Regina v. T (2010)
Dawid (2001), Bayes’s Theorem and Weighing Evidence by Juries
Additional readings
Fenton (2011), Improve Statistics in Court
Hamer (2012), The R v T Controversy: Forensic Evidence, Law and Logic
Urbaniak and Di Bello (2021), Legal Probabilism, Sec. 1 and 2
9/26
Week 6: Probability and decisions
Once the evidence has been presented and evaluated, a decision must be made. To this end, legal standard of proof serves as decision criteria, for example, convict only if the defendant’s guilt was established beyond a reasonable doubt. Since these standards are not precise, legal probabilists have translated them in the more precise language of probability and statistics. The usefulness of this translation is debated because eliminating all reasonable doubts about someone’s guilt seems to require more than establishing a high probability of guilt.
Primary readings
Smith (2017), When Does Evidence Suffice for Conviction?
Gardiner (2019), The Reasonable and the Relevant: Legal Standards of Proof
Additional readings
Kaplan (1968), Decision Theory and the Factfinding Process
Redmayne (2008), Exploring the Proof Paradoxes
10/3
Week 7: Knowledge and legal proof (remote class)
Epistemologists are concerned with knowledge and its value. Knowledge requires more than high probability, as lottery propositions suggest. Could the notion of knowledge help us better understand the standards of proof in legal cases?
Primary readings
- Moss (2023), Knowledge and Legal Proof
Additional readings
- Papineau (2021), The Disvalue of Knowledge
10/10
First essay due
Week 8: Relative plausibility
A prominent alternative to the probabilistic approach is the theory of relative plausibility. On this theory, trial decisions are guided by comparing the relative plausibility of two competing explanations of the evidence. The explanations are put forward by the parties at trial and the more plausible should be prevail. This comparative approach tames the complexity of trial decision-making in which alternative possibilities about what could have happened are potentially infinite in number and cannot be examined in their entirety.
Q&A with Ronald Allen
The first part of the class will consist in a Q&A session with Ronald Allen, a legal scholar of evidence law, criminal procedure, and constitutional law. He was recently retained by the Tanzanian Government to assist in the reform of their evidence law. Ronald is the author of one of the readings assigned for today.
Primary readings
Pardo and Allen (2007), Juridical Proof and Inference and the Best Explanatiopn
Allen (2017), The Nature of Juridical Proof: Probability as a Tool in Plausible Reasoning
Additional readings
Steele and Stefansson (2021), Belief Revision for Growing Awareness
Dahlman (2024), What Does it Mean to Be Plausible?
10/17
Week 9: The story model
A close cousin of the theory of relative plausibility is the story model. According to this model, judges and jurors evaluate the evidence presented at trial not in a piecemeal manner, but holistically by constructing a unifying narrative or story of what happened. Can probability theory model this holistic process? Bayesian networks have been used to calculate numerically the weight of complex bodies of evidence. We will discuss the prospects of using Bayesian networks for evaluating evidence in trial proceedings.
Q&A with Mackor, Dahlman and Lagnado
The first part of the class will consist in a Q&A session with Anne Ruth Mackor, Christian Dahlman, and David Lagnado. The first two are legal scholars and philosophers, and the third is a cognitive psychologist. They are interested in understanding how judges and jurors assess evidence in conditions of uncertainty. They are the authors of some of the readings assigned for today.
Primary readings
Pennington and Hastie (1993), ‘The Story Model for Juror Decision Making’, in Inside the Juror: The Psychology of Juror Decision Making - check canvas
Fenton and other (2019), Analyzing the Simonshaven Case Using Bayesian Networks
Additional readings
Dahlman and Mackor (2019), Coherence and Probability in Legal Evidence
Urbaniak and Di Bello (2021), Legal Probabilism, Sec. 3
10/24
Week 10: The Conjunction Paradox
Legal cases often require that multiple propositions be established. For example, to establish bribery, a prosecutor needs to establish several elements: (i) a public official received the bribe; (ii) the bribe was something of value; (iii) the public official carried out an official act because of the bribe; (iv) the bribing party had intent to bribe. Can probability and statistics help to put them together, and yield a unified picture? This is where legal probabilism faces significant challenges, one of the most discussed being the difficulty about conjunction.
Q&A with Emily Spottswood
The first part of the class will consist in a Q&A session with Emily Spottswood, a legal scholar interested in how reforms to the rules of procedure and evidence can make civil and criminal litigation more accurate and cost-effective. Emily is the author of one of the readings assigned for today.
Primary readings
Cohen (1977), ‘The Difficulty about Conjunction’, in The Probable and The Provable - check Canvas
Spottswood (2016), Unraveling the Conjunction Paradox
Additional readings
Dawid (1987), The Difficulty About Conjunction
Haack (2014), Legal Probabilism: An Epistemological Dissent
10/31
Second essay due
Week 11: Beyond the witness?
When evidence is presented at trial, it will be scrutinized and tested against objections by both parties. This cross-examination is directed at witnesses, so their live testimonies play a central role. But new developments in technology have challenged this testimony-centered approach. How to handle machine evidence, such as risk assessments, AI diagnostic tools, or softwares that interpret DNA evidence? Should the testimony-centered approach be abandoned?
Q&A with Ed Cheng and Alex Nun
The first part of the class will consist in a Q&A session with Ed Cheng, a legal scholar at interested in expert evidence, law and statistics, and remedies, and Alex Nun, a legal scholars interested in how evidentiary rules and the allocation of decisionmaking authority in the courtroom affect verdict accuracy, efficiency, and legitimacy. Ed and Alex are the authors of one of the readings assigned for today.
Primary readings
Cheng and Nunn (2019), Beyond the Witness
Roth (2017), Machine Testimony
Additional readings
11/07
Week 12: Eyewitness identifications
Eyewitness testimony is often the central piece of a prosecutor’s case against the defendant. Eyewitness evidence is not numerical on its face—it consists of qualitative statements “I saw this or that”. However, a growing body of research in psychology and law has deployed the tools of probability and statistics to assess the value of eyewitness evidence. While some studies emphasize its many pitfalls, more recent studies offer a more nuanced picture.
Primary readings
Loftus and Palmer (1974), Reconstruction of Auto-mobile Destruction: An example of the Interaction Between Language and Memory
Wixted and Wells (2017), The Relationship Between Eyewitness Confidence and Identification Accuracy: A New Synthesis
Additional readings
11/14
Week 13: Wrongful convictions
We have examined how probability and statistics help to evaluate evidence, piecemeal or in the aggregate, and how they can guide trial decisions. But they can also be used as tools for assessing how the trial system performs as a whole, for example, how accurate it is and how often innocent people are convicted. Some estimate that the rate of wrongful convictions is about 3-5%.
Primary readings
- Risinger (2007), Innocents Convicted
Additional readings
Simon (2011), Limited Diagnosticity of Criminal Trials
Sangero and Halpert (2007), Why a Conviction Should Not Be Based on a Single Piece of Evidence
11/21
Week 14: Workshop
Students will workshop their research papers by giving short presentations accompanied by either a handout or slides and followed by Q&A. Each presenter will have 30 minutes to be split between the presentation itself and Q&A at the discretion of the presenter. The list of presenters is TBA.
11/28
Week 15: Thanksgiving break
12/5
Week 16: Conclusion
What have we learned? What questions are still left open and what lines of inquiry seemed more promising? Is legal probabilism a good idea or a big mistake?
Primary readings
Tribe, Trial by Methmatics
Saks and Neufeld (2011), Convergent Evolution in Law and Science: the Structure of Decision-making under Uncertainty
Additional readings
Harcourt (2018), The Systems Fallacy: A Genealogy and Critique of Public Policy and Cost-Benefit Analysis
Hedden and Colyvan (2019), Legal Probabilism: A Qualified Defence
Third essay or research paper due December 5th
Title IX
Title IX is a federal law that provides that no person be excluded on the basis of sex from participation in, be denied benefits of, or be subjected to discrimination under any education program or activity. Both Title IX and university policy make clear that sexual violence and harassment based on sex is prohibited. An individual who believes they have been subjected to sexual violence or harassed on the basis of sex can seek support, including counseling and academic support, from the university. If you or someone you know has been harassed on the basis of sex or sexually assaulted, you can find information and resources at https://sexualviolenceprevention.asu.edu/faqs.
As a mandated reporter, I am obligated to report any information I become aware of regarding alleged acts of sexual discrimination, including sexual violence and dating violence. ASU Counseling Services, https://eoss.asu.edu/counseling is available if you wish to discuss any concerns confidentially and privately. ASU online students may access 360 Life Services, https://goto.asuonline.asu.edu/success/online-resources.html.